Ãëàâíàÿ Ñëó÷àéíàÿ ñòðàíèöà


Ïîëåçíîå:

Êàê ñäåëàòü ðàçãîâîð ïîëåçíûì è ïðèÿòíûì Êàê ñäåëàòü îáúåìíóþ çâåçäó ñâîèìè ðóêàìè Êàê ñäåëàòü òî, ÷òî äåëàòü íå õî÷åòñÿ? Êàê ñäåëàòü ïîãðåìóøêó Êàê ñäåëàòü òàê ÷òîáû æåíùèíû ñàìè çíàêîìèëèñü ñ âàìè Êàê ñäåëàòü èäåþ êîììåð÷åñêîé Êàê ñäåëàòü õîðîøóþ ðàñòÿæêó íîã? Êàê ñäåëàòü íàø ðàçóì çäîðîâûì? Êàê ñäåëàòü, ÷òîáû ëþäè îáìàíûâàëè ìåíüøå Âîïðîñ 4. Êàê ñäåëàòü òàê, ÷òîáû âàñ óâàæàëè è öåíèëè? Êàê ñäåëàòü ëó÷øå ñåáå è äðóãèì ëþäÿì Êàê ñäåëàòü ñâèäàíèå èíòåðåñíûì?


Êàòåãîðèè:

ÀðõèòåêòóðàÀñòðîíîìèÿÁèîëîãèÿÃåîãðàôèÿÃåîëîãèÿÈíôîðìàòèêàÈñêóññòâîÈñòîðèÿÊóëèíàðèÿÊóëüòóðàÌàðêåòèíãÌàòåìàòèêàÌåäèöèíàÌåíåäæìåíòÎõðàíà òðóäàÏðàâîÏðîèçâîäñòâîÏñèõîëîãèÿÐåëèãèÿÑîöèîëîãèÿÑïîðòÒåõíèêàÔèçèêàÔèëîñîôèÿÕèìèÿÝêîëîãèÿÝêîíîìèêàÝëåêòðîíèêà






Chapter III linguistic features of Germanic languages





Preliminary Remarks (§ 50)

§ 50. All the Germanic languages of the past and present have com­mon linguistic features; some of these features are shared by other groups in the IE family, others are specifically Germanic.

The Germanic group acquired their specific distinctive features after the separation of the ancient Germanic tribes from other IE tribes and prior to their further expansion and disintegration, that is during the period of the PG parent-language. These PG features inherited by the descendant languages, represent the common features of the Ger­manic group. Other common features developed later, in the course of the individual histories of separate Germanic languages, as a result of similar tendencies arising from PG causes. On the other hand, many Germanic features have been disguised, transformed and even lost in later history.

PHONETICS (§ 51-61)

Word Stress (§ 51-52)

§ 51. The peculiar Germanic system of word accentuation is one of the most important distinguishing features of the group; it arose in PG, was fully or partly retained in separate languages and served as one of the major causes for many linguistic changes.

It is known that in ancient IE, prior to the separation of Germanic, there existed two ways of word accentuation: musical pitch and force stress. The position of the stress was free and movable, which means that it could fall on any syllable of the word — a root-morpheme, an affix or an ending — and could be shifted both in form-building and word-building (cf. R äóìîì, äîìá, äîìîâíè÷àòü, äóìà).

Both these properties of the word accent were changed in PG. Force or expiratory stress (also called dynamic and breath stress) became the only type of stress used. In Early PG word stress was still as movable as in ancient IE but in Late PG its position in the word was stabilised. The stress was now fixed on the first syllable, which was usually the root of the word and sometimes the prefix; the other syllables — suffixes and endings — were unstressed. The stress could no longer move either in form-building or word-building.

These features of word accent were inherited by the Germanic lan­guages, and despite later alterations are observable today. In Mod E there is a sharp contrast between accented and unaccented syllables due to the force of the stress. The main accent commonly falls on the root-morpheme, and is never shifted in building grammatical forms. The following English and German words illustrate its fixed position in grammatical forms and derived words:

English: be'come, be'coming, over'come; 'lover, 'loving, be'loved;

German: 'Liebe, 'lieben 'lieble, ge'liebt, 'lieberhaft, 'Liebling.

(Cf. these native words with words of foreign origin which move the stress in derivation, though never in form-building: ex'hibit v, exhibition n).

§ 52. The heavy fixed word stress inherited from PG has played an important role in the development of the Germanic languages, and especially in phonetic and morphological changes. Due to the difference in the force of articulation the stressed and unstressed syllables under­went widely different changes: accented syllables were pronounced with great distinctness and precision, while unaccented became less distinct and were phonetically weakened. The differences between the sounds in stressed position were preserved and emphasised, whereas the con­trasts between the unaccented sounds were weakened and lost. Since the stress was fixed on the root, the weakening and loss of sounds mainly affected the suffixes and grammatical endings. Many endings merged with the suffixes, were weakened and dropped. Cf., e.g., the reconstructed PG word ‘fish’, with its descendants in Old Germanic languages:

PG *fiskaz, Gt fisks, O Icel fiskr, OE fisc.

(The asterisk * is placed before reconstructed hypothetical forms which have not been found in written records; the words may be pro­nounced exactly as they are written; spelling in Old Germanic languages was phonetic).

Vowels (§ 53-56)

§ 53. Throughout history, beginning with PG, vowels displayed a strong tendency to change. They underwent different kinds of altera­tions: qualitative and quantitative, dependent and independent. Qualitative changes affect the quality of the sound, e.g.: [o > a ] or [p > f]; quantitative changes make long sounds short or short sounds long, e.g.: [i > i:], [ll > l]; dependent changes (also positional or combinative)are re­stricted to certain positions or phonetic conditions, for instance, a sound may change under the influence of the neighbouring sounds or in a certain type of a syllable; independent changes — also spontaneous or regular — take place irrespective of phonetic conditions, i.e. they affect a certain sound in all positions.

From an early date the treatment of vowels was determined by the nature of word stress. In accented syllables the oppositions between vowels were carefully maintained and new distinctive features were introduced, so that the number of stressed vowels grew. In unaccented positions the original contrasts between vowels were weakened or lost; the distinction of short and long vowels was neutralised so that by the age of writing the long vowels in unstressed syllables had been shortened. As for originally short vowels, they tended to be reduced to a neutral sound, losing their qualitative distinctions and were often dropped in unstressed final syllables (see the example *fiskaz in § 52).

§ 54. Strict differentiation of long and short vowels is commonly regarded as an important characteristic of the Germanic group. The contrast of short and long vowels is supported by the different directions of their changes. While long vowels generally tended to become closer and to diphthongise, short vowels, on the contrary, often changed into more open sounds. These tendencies can be seen in the earliest vowel changes which distinguished the PG vowel system from its PIE source.

IE short [o] changed in Germanic into the more open vowel [a] and thus ceased to be distinguished from the original IE [a]; in other words inPG they merged into [o]. The merging of long vowels proceeded inthe opposite direction: IE long [a:] was narrowed to [o:] and merged with [o:]. The examples in Table 1 illustrate the resulting correspond­ences of vowels in parallels from Germanic and non-Germanic languages (more apparent in Old Germanic languages than inmodern words, for the sounds have been modified in later history).

Table 1

Independent Vowel Changes in Proto-Germanic

Change illustrated Examples
PIE PG Non-Germanic Germanic
      Old Modern
o   a   L nox, Ir nochd, R íî÷ü R ìîãó; ìî÷ü Gt nahts, Î Icel natt, OHG naht Gt magan, OE maʒan, mæʒ Sw natt, G Nacht   Sw ma, NE may
a: o: L mater, R ìàòü   O Ind bhrata, L frater, R áðàò O Icel moðir, OE modor Gt bropar, O Icel broðir, OE broðor Sw moder, NE mother Sw broder, NE brother

§ 55. In later PG and in separate Germanic languages the vowels displayed a tendency to positional assimilative changes: the pronunciation of a vowel was modified under the influence of the following or preceding consonant; sometimes a vowel was approximated more closely to the following vowel. The resulting sounds were phonetically conditioned allophones which could eventually coincide with another phoneme or develop into a new phoneme.

The earliest instances of progressive assimilation were common Germanic muta­tions; they occurred in Late PG before its disintegration or a short time after. In certain phonetic conditions, namely before the nasal [n] and before [i] or [j] in the next syllable the short [e], [j] and [u] remained orbecame close (i.e. appeared as [i] and [u]), while in the absence of these conditions the more open allophones were used; [e] and [o], respectively. Later, these phonetic conditions became irre­levant and the allophones were phonologised.

Table 2

Mutation of Vowels in Late PG

Change illustrated Examples
Non-Germanic Germanic
Old Modern
PIE G L ventus, R âåòåð Gt winds, O Icel vindr, OE wind Sw vind, NE w ind
i e e L edit, R åñò   L edere, R åñòü OHG izit, OE itep, O Icel eta, OE etan G iβt, NE eats, G essen, NE eat
u u o Lith sunus, R ñûí Celt human O Icel sunr, OE sunu O Icel, OE horn Sw son, NE son NE horn, Sw horn

§ 56. After the changes, in Late PG, the vowel system contained the following sounds:

SHORT VOWELS i e a o u

LONG VOWELS i: e: a: o: u:[5]

It is believed that in addition to these monophthongs PG had a set of diphthongs made up of more open nuclei and closer glides: [ei], [ai]. [eu], [au] and also [iu]; nowadays, however, many scholars inter­pret them as sequences of two independent monophthongs.

Consonants. Proto-germanic consonant shift (§ 57-59)

§ 57. The specific peculiarities of consonants constitute the most re­markable distinctive feature of the Germanic linguistic group. Compa­rison with other languages within the IE family reveals regular cor­respondences between Germanic and non-Germanic consonants. Thus we regularly find [f] in Germanic where other IE languages have [p]; cf. e.g., E full,R ïîëíûé,Fr plein;wherever Germanic has [p], cognate words in non-Germanic languages have [b] (cf. E pool, R áîëîòî). The consonants in Germanic look ‘shifted’ as compared with the consonants of non-Germanic languages. The alterations of the con­sonants took place in PG, and the resulting sounds were inherited by the languages of the Germanic group.

The changes of consonants in PG were first formulated in terms of a phonetic law by Jacob Grimm in the early 19th c. and are often called Grimm's Law. It is also known as the First or Proto-Germanic consonant shift (to be distinguished from the 2nd shift which took place in OHG in the 9th c.).

By the terms of Grimm's Law voiceless plosives developed in PG into voiceless fricatives (Act I); IE voiced plosives were shifted to voice­less plosives (Act II) and IE voiced aspirated plosives were reflected (See Note 1 to Table 3) either as voiced fricatives or as pure voiced plo­sives (Act III).

Table 3

Consonant. Shift in Proto-Germanic (Grimm's Law)

Correspondence illustrated Examples  
Non-Germanic Germanic  
Old Modern  
PIE PG        
ACT I        
p f L pes, pedis Gt fotus, O Icel fotr, OE fot Sw fot, NE foot G Fuβ  
    R ïåíà OE fam G Feim, NE foam  
    L piscis, R ïåñêàðü Gt fisks, OE fisc G Fisch, NE fish  
t θ L tres, R òðè Gt preis, O Icel prir, OE preo Sw tre, G drei, NE three  
    L tu, Fr tu, R òû Gt pu, OE pu G Sw du, NE thou  
k x L cor, cordis, Fr coeur, R ñåðäöå Gt hairto, O Icel hjarta, OE heort G Herz, NE h eart  
    L canis R êîëîäà Gt hunds, OE hund OE holt G Hund, NE hound G Holz, NE holt  
ACT II        
b p Lith bala, R áîëîòî OHG pfuol, OE pol G Pfuhl, NE pool  
    L labare, R ñëàáûé Gt slepan, OE slæpan G schlafen, NE steep  
d t L decem, Fr dix, R äåñÿòü Gt taihun, O Icel tiu, OE tien Sw tio, G zehn, NE ten  
    Fr deux, R äâà OF twa NE two  
    L edere, R åäà Gt itan, OE etan Sw ata, NE ear  
    L. videre, R âåäàòü, âèäåòü OF witan G wissen, NE wit  
g k L genu, Fr genou L ager OE cneo, Gt kniu Gt akrs, O Icel akr, OE æcer NE knee, G Knie Sw aker, NE acre  
    L iugum, R èãî Gt juk, O Icel ok, OE ʒeoc Sw ok, NE yoke  
ACT III      
bh[6] v (or b) O Ind bhrata, L frater, R áðàò Gt bropar, O Icel bróðir, OE bropor Sw broder, G Bruder, NE brother
    L ferre, R áåðó Gt bairan, OE beran G gebären, NE bear
    F r future, R áûòü OHG bin, bist, OE bēon G bin, bist, NE be
dh ð (or d) O Ind rudhira, R ðäåòü Gt raups, O Icel rauðr, OE rēad G rot, Sw röd, NE red
    O Ind mádhyas, L medius Gt midjis [ð], OE middel G Mittel, NE middle
    R äåëàòü Gt gadeps, OE dæd, dōn NE deed, do
gh γ (or g) L hostis, R ãîñòü Gt gasts, O Icel gestr, OE giest Sw g ä st, G Gast, NE guest
    L (leg-) lectus, R çàëåãàòü Gt li gan [γ], O Icel liggja, OE licʒan G liegen, NE lie
    O Ind vaha, L via, R âåçòè Gt wiga [γ], O Icel vegr, OE weʒ Sw väg, G Weg, NE way
               

§ 58. Another important series of consonant changes in PG was discovered in the late 19th c. by a Danish scholar, Carl Verner. They are known as Verner's Law. Vemer's Law explains some correspondences of consonants which seemed to contradict Grimm's Law and were for a long time regarded as exceptions. According to Verner's Law all the early PG voiceless fricatives [f, θ, x] which arose under Grimm's Law, and also is) inherited from PIE, became voiced between vowels if the preceding vowel was unstressed; in the absence of these conditions they remained voiceless. The voicing occurred in early PG at the time when the stress was not yet fixed on the root-morpheme. The process of voicing can be shown as a step in a succession of consonant changes in prehistorical reconstructed forms; consider, e.g. the changes of the second consonant in the word father:

PIE Early PG Late PG
*pa'ter > *fa'θar > *fa'ðar > >*'faðar

Verner's Law accounts for the appearance of voiced fricative or its later modifications [d] in place of the voiceless [θ] which ought to be expected under Grimm's Law. In late PG, the phonetic conditions that caused the voicing had disappeared: the stress had shifted to the first syllable.

Table 4

Voicing of Fricatives in Proto-Germanic (Verner's Law)

Change illustrated Examples
PIE PG Non-Germanic Germanic
  Early Late   old modern
p f > v L caput Gt haubip, O Icel haufoð, OE hēafod [v] Sw huvud, G Haupt, NE head
    L septem Gt sibun, OE seofon [v] G sieben, NE seven
t θ >ð,d O Ind satam, R ñòî Gt hund, O Icel hundrað, OE hund G Hundert, Sw hundrade, NE hundred
    L pater, O Ind pita Gt fadar [ð], O Icel faðir, OE fæder G Vater, Sw fader, NE father
k x > γ,g L cunctāari O Icel hanga, OE hanʒian Sw h ä nga, NE hang
    L socrus, R ñâåêðîâü Gt swaihro, OHG swigur, OE sweser G Schwager
s s > z L auris, Lith ausis Gt auso, O Icel eyra, OE ēare Swöra, G Ohr, NE ear
(Note: [z] in many languages became [r])  

§ 59. As a result at voicing by Verner's Law there arose an interchange of consonants in the grammatical forms of the word, termed grammatical interchange. Part of the forms retained a voiceless fricative, while other forms — with a diffe­rent position of stress in Early PG — acquired a voiced fricative. Both consonants could undergo later changes in the OG languages, but the original difference be­tween them goes back to the time of movable word stress and PG voicing. The interchanges can be seen in the principal forms of some OG verbs, though even at that time most of the interchanges were levelled out by analogy.

Table 5

Grammatical Interchanges of Consonants caused by Verner's Law

Interchange Principal forms of the verbs
PG OG languages Infini­tive Past Tense Participle II NE
sg pl
f ~ v OHG f ~ b heffen huob huobun gi-haban heave
ð~ θ OE ð/θ ~ d sēoðan sēað sudon soden seethe
x ~ y O Icel, slá sló slógum sleginn  
  OE x ~ y slēan   slōʒon slæʒen slay
s ~ z OE s/z ~ r cēosan cēas curon coren choose

Note that some Mod E words have retained traces of Verner's Law, e. g. seethe — sodden; death — dead; raise — rear; was — were.

Interpretation of the Proto-Germanic Consonant Shift (§ 60-61)

§ 60. The causes and mechanism of the PG consonant shift have been a matter of discussion ever since the shift was discovered.

When Jacob Grimm first formulated the law of the shift he ascribed it to the allegedly daring spirit of the Germanic tribes which manifested itself both in their great migrations and in radical linguistic innovations. His theory has long been rejected as naive and romantic.

Some philologists attributed the shift to the physiological peculiarities of the Teutons, namely the shape of their glottis: it differed from that of other IB tribes, and the pronunciation of consonants was modified. Other scholars maintained that the consonant shift was caused by a more energetic articulation of sounds brought about by the specifically Germanic force word stress. Another theory suggested that the articulation of consonants in Germanic was, on the contrary, marked by lack of energy and tension.

The theory of "linguistic substratum" which was popular with many 20th c. linguists, attributes the PG consonant changes — as well as other Germanic inno­vations — to the influence of the speech habits of pre-Germanic population in the areas of Germanic settlement. The language of those unknown tribes served as a sort ol substratum (‘under-layer’) for the would-be Germanic tongues; it intermixed with the language of the Teutons and left certain traces in PG. This hypothesis can be neither confirmed nor disproved, since we possess no intamation about the language of pre-IE inhabitants of Western Europe.

According to recent theories the PG consonant shift could be caused by the internal requirements of the language system: the need for more precise phonemic distinction reliable in all phonetic conditions. Before the shift (according to J. Kurylowicz) the opposition of voiced and voiceless plosives was neutralised (that is, lost) in some positions, namely before the sound [s]; therefore new distinctive features arose in place of or in addition to sonority. [p, t, k] changed into [f, 0, x] and began to be contrasted to [b, d, g] not only through sonority but also through the manner of articulation as fricatives to plosives. This change led to further changes: since [f, 0, x]were now opposed to [b, d, g] through their frica­tive character, sonority became irrelevant for phonemic distinction and [b, d, g ]were devoiced: they changed into [p, t, k], respectively. That is how the initial step stimulated further changes and the entire system was shifted. It is essential that throughout the shift the original pattern of the consonant system was preserved; three rows of noise consonants were distinguished, though instead of opposition through sonority consonants were opposed as fricatives to plosives. (For a critical review of various theories see «Ñðàâíèòåëüíàÿ ãðàììàòèêà ãåðìàíñêèõ ÿçûêîâ", M., 1962, êí. II, ÷. I, ãë. 1, 7.1 — 6.5.)

Another explanation based on the structural approach to language interprets the role of the language system from a different angle. Every subsystem in lan­guage tends to preserve a balanced, symmetrical arrangement: if the balance is bro­ken, it will soon be restored by means of new changes. After the replacement of [p, t, k] by [f, 0, k] the positions of the voiceless [p, t, k] in the consonant system were left vacant; to fill the vacuums and restore the equilibrium [b, d, g ]were devoiced into [p, t, k]. In their turn the vacant positions of [b, d, g] were filled again in the succeeding set of changes, when [bh, dh, gh] lost their aspirated character. This theory, showing the shift as a chain of successive steps, fails to account for the initial push.

§ 61. The chronology of the shift and the relative order of the changes includ­ed in Grimm's Law and Verner's Law, has also aroused much interest and specu­lation. It is believed that the consonant shift was realised as a series of successive steps; it began first on part of Germanic territories and gradually spread over the whole area. The change of [p, t, k] into fricatives is unanimously regarded as the earliest step — the first act of Grimm's Law; it was followed, or, perhaps, accom­panied by the voicing of fricatives (Verner's Law), Linguists of the 19th c. were inclined to refer the voicing of fricatives to a far later date than the first act of Grimm's Law, However, there are no grounds to think that the effect of word stress and intervocal position on sonority could have been much delayed. In all probability, the IE plosives split into voiced and voiceless sounds soon after they had acquired their fricative character or even during that process.

The order of the other two steps (or acts of Grimm's Law) varies in different descriptions of the shift.

GRAMMAR (§ 62-69)

Form-building Means (§ 62)

§ 62. Like other old IE languages both PG and the OG languages had a synthetic grammatical structure, which means that the relationships between the parts of the sentence were shown by the forms of the words rather than by their position or by auxiliary words. In later history all the Germanic languages developed analytical forms and ways of word connection.

In the early periods of history the grammatical forms were built in the synthetic way: by means of inflections, sound interchanges and suppletion.

The suppletive way of form-building was inherited from ancient IE, it was restricted to a few personal pronouns, adjectives and verbs.

Compare the following forms of pronouns in Germanic and non-Germanic languages:

L Fr R Gt O Icel OE NE
ego je ÿ ik ek ic I
mei mon ìåíÿ meina min min my, mine
mihi me, moi ìíå mis mer me me

The principal means of form-building were inflections. The inflec­tions found in OG written records correspond to the inflections used in non-Germanic languages, having descended from the same original IE prototypes. Most of them, however, were simpler and shorter, as they had been shortened and weakened in PG.

The wide use of sound interchanges has always been a characteristic feature of the Germanic group. This form-building (and word-building) device was inherited from IE and became very productive in Germanic. In various forms of the word and in words derived from one and the same root, the root-morpheme appeared as a set of variants. The consonants were relatively stable, the vowels were variable[7]. Table 6 shows the variability of the root *ber- in different grammatical forms and words.

Table 6

Variants of the Root *ber-

  Old Germanic languages Modern Germanic languages
  Gt O Icel OE Sw G NE
forms of the verb bear bairan bera beran bära gebären bear
bar bar bær bar gebar bore (sg)
berum bárum bæron buro (Pi)
baurans borinn boren buren geboren born
    birp     bears
other words from the -ame root barn barn bearn barn Geburt barn (dial, ‘child’)
baur burðr ʒebyrd     birth
  byrð        

Vowel Gradation with Special Reference to Verbs (§ 63-65)

§ 63. Vowel interchanges found in Old and Modern Germanic lan­guages originated at different historical periods. The earliest set of vowel interchanges, which dates from PG and PIE, is called vowel grada­tion or ablaut. Ablaut is an independent vowel interchange unconnected with any phonetic conditions; different vowels appear in the same en­vironment, surrounded by the same sounds (all the words in Table 6 are examples of ablaut with the exception of the forms containing [i] and [y] which arose from positional changes.

Vowel gradation did not reflect any phonetic changes but was used as a special independent device to differentiate between words and gram­matical forms built from the same root.

Ablaut was inherited by Germanic from ancient IE. The principal gradation series used in the IE languages — [e ~ o] — can be shown in Russian examples: íåñòè ~ íîøà. This kind of ablaut is called qual­itative, as the vowels differ only in quality. Alternation of short and long vowels, and also alternation with a "zero" (i.e. lack of vowel) re­present quantitative ablaut:

Prolonged grade (long vowel) Normal or lull grade (short vowel) Reduced grade (zero grade) (neutral vowel or loss of vowel)
ē e  
L lēgi ‘elected’ lego ‘elect’  
R — e ~ o  
  áåðó — ñáîð áðàë

The Germanic languages employed both types of ablaut — quali­tative and quantitative, — and their combinations. In accordance with vowel changes which distinguished Germanic from non-Germanic the gradation series were modified: IE [e ~ o] was changed to [e/i ~ a]; likewise, quantitative ablaut [a ~a:] was reflected in Germanic as a quantitative-qualitative series [a ~ o:] (for relevant vowel changes see § 53,54). Quantitative ablaut gave rise to a variety of gradation series in Germanic owing to different treatment of the zero-grade in various phonetic conditions.

§ 64. Of all its spheres of application in Germanic ablaut was most consistently used in building the principal forms of the verbs called strong. Each form was characterised by a certain grade; each set of principal forms of the verb employed a gradation series. Gradation vowels were combined with other sounds in different classes of verbs and thus yielded several new gradation series. The use of ablaut in the principal forms of ‘bear’ was shown in Table 6. The Gothic verbs in Table 6 give the closest possible approximation to PG gradation series, which were inherited by all the OG languages and were modified in ac­cordance with later phonetic changes (see OE strong verbs § 200-203)

Table 7

Examples of Vowel Gradation in Gothic Strong Verbs

IE e   zero zero
PG e/i a zero zero
Principal forms Infinitive Past sg Past pl Participle II
Class 1 reisan rais risum risans NE rise
Class 2 kiusan kaus kusum kusans choose
Class 3 bindan band bundum bundans bind

§ 65. The use of ablaut in the sphere of grammar was not confined to the root-vowels of strong verbs. The gradation series [e/i ~ a] accounts for the interchange of vowels in some grammatical endings in the noun and verb paradigms. This gradation series is found, e.g. in the following noun-endings: PG Nom. sg — *-az, Gen.sg -*eso/-iso (the vowels represent different grades of ablaut of the suffix -a — see § 67). The same series [e/i ~ a] is found in the endings of many verbs (called thematic in contrast to athematic verbs, which did not contain any vo­calic element), e.g. Present Tense —

2nd p. sg Gt -is — OE -est
3rd p. sg Gt -ip OE -(i) p
  pl Gt -and OE -a ð

Simplification of Word Structure in Late Proto-Germanic. Role of Stem-suffixes in the Formation of Declensions (§ 66-67)

§ 66. Some changes in the morphological structure of the word in Late PG account for the development of an elaborate system of declen­sions in OG languages, and for the formation of grammatical endings.

Originally, in Early PG theword consisted of three main component parts: Theroot, the stem-suffix and the grammatical ending. The stem-suffix was a means of word derivation, the ending — a marker of the grammatical form. In Late PG the old stem-suffixes lost their deriva­tional force and merged with other components of the word, usually with the endings. The word was simplified: the three-morpheme struc­ture was transformed into a two-morpheme structure. The original gram­matical ending, together with the stem-suffix formed a new ending:

(In Gt fisks the stem-suffix was dropped, in OE macian, macode it merged with the ending, preserving one of the sounds — [i] or [o].) The simplification of the word structure and the loss of stem-suffixes as distinct components was facilitated — or, perhaps, caused — by the heavy Germanic word stress fixed on the root.

§ 67. Most nouns and adjectives in PG, and also many verbs, had stem-forming suffixes; according to stem-suffixes they fell into groups, or classes: a-stems, f-stems, ō-stems, etc. This grouping accounts for the formation of different declensions in nouns and adjectives, and for some differences in the conjugation of verbs.

Groups of nouns with different stem-suffixes made distinct types of declension. The original grammatical endings were alike for most nouns, e.g. Norn, sg -z, Dat. -i, Acc. -m. When these endings fused with different stem-suffixes, each group of nouns acquired a different set of endings. The division of nouns into declensions resting on the stem-suf­fixes is not peculiar to Germanic alone; it is also found in other IE lan­guages (some types of declension in Germanic correspond to certain de­clensions in non-Germanic languages, e.g. ō - stems correspond to the first declension in Latin and Russian (their stem-suffix is -a: Germanic -ō has developed from IE -ā; Germanic a-stems correspond to the second declension in Latin and in Russian (ō-stems in both these languages, since IE [o] became [ a ] in Germanic).

The Germanic languages preserved the old classification of nouns with great accuracy, added other distinctive features to the noun para­digms and, as a result, had a complicated system of noun declensions in the early periods of history.

Strong and Weak Verbs (§ 68-69)

§ 68. The bulk of the verbs in PG and in the OG languages fall into two large groups called strong and weak.

The terms strong and weak were proposed by J. Grimm; he called the verbs strong because they had preserved the richness of form since the age of the parent-language and in this sense could be contrasted to weak verbs lacking such variety of form. From the verbs the terms were extended to noun and adjective declensions. The main difference between these groups lies in the means of building the principal forms: the Pres­ent tense, the Past tense and Participle II. The strong verbs built their principal forms with the help of root vowel interchanges plus certain grammatical endings; they made use of IE ablaut with certain modifi­cations due to phonetic changes and environment (see § 64).

§69. The weak verbs are a specifically Germanic innovation, for the device used in building their principal forms is not found outside the Germanic group. They built the Past tense and Participle 11 by inserting a special suffix between the root and the ending. The suffix — PG -ð — is referred to as the dental suffix, as [ð] is an interdental fric­ative consonant. The use of the dental suffix is seen in the following forms of weak verbs in OG languages:

  Infinitive Past Tense Participle II NE
Gt domjan domida [ð] domips deem, deemed
O Icel kalla kallaða kallaðr call, called
OE macian macode macod make, made

The dental suffix [ð, 0, d] is a marker of the Past and Participle II; it is preceded by remnants of the old srem-suffixes: -i- in the Gt domida, -o- in OE macode. The weak verbs formed several classes with different stem-suffixes, in the same way as nouns fell into declensions.

VOCABULARY (§ 70-72)

§ 70. Until recently it was believed that the Germanic languages had a large proportion of words, which have no parallels in other groups of the IE family. Recent research, however, has revealed numerous non-Germanic parallels for words formerly regarded as specifically Germanic. It appears that Germanic has inherited and preserved many IE features inlexis as well as at other levels.

The most ancient etymological layer in the Germanic vocabulary is made up of words (or, more precisely, roots) shared by most IE lan­guages. They refer to a number of semantic spheres: natural phenomena, plants and animals, terms of kinship, verbs denoting basic activities of man, some pronouns and numerals; in addition to roots, the common IE element includes other components of words: word-building affixes and grammatical inflections. Numerous examples of parallels belong­ing to this layer were quoted above, to show the sound correspondences in Germanic and non-Germanic languages (§ 54, 55, 57, 58).

§ 71. Words which occur in Germanic alone and have no parallels outside the group constitute the specific features of the Germanic lan­guages; they appeared inPG or inlater history of separate languages from purely Germanic roots. Semantically, they also belong to basic spheres of life: nature, sea, home life. Like the IE layer the specifically Germanic layer includes not only roots but also affixes and word-build­ing patterns. The examples in Table 8 illustrate Germanic words, whose roots have not been found outside the group, and some word-building patterns which arose in Late PG. Those are instances of transition from compound words into derived words; they show the development of new suffixes — from root-morphemes — at the time when many old deri­vational stem-suffixes had lost their productivity and ceased to be distin­guished in the word structure. The new suffixes made up for the loss of stem-suffixes.

(Though some of these words have no direct descendants we find the same suffixes in other formations: G Irrtum, Freundschaft, NE friend­ship — the roots may be common IE but the suffixes are specifically Germanic (vis- is related to R âåäàòü, fr ē ond to R ïðèÿòåëü ) .

Table 8

Specifically Germanic Words and Word Building Patterns

Old Germanic languages Modern Germanic languages
Gt O Iñel OHG OE   G NE
bus hús hûs hūs hus Haus house
drigkan drekka trinkan drincan dricka trinken drink
land land lant land land Land land
saiws sær sëo sjö See sea
visd ó mr wistuom wisdōm wisdom wisdom
  fjands- kapr fiands- caft feond- scipe Fiend- skap Feind- schaft ‘hostility’ (cf. fiend)

§ 72. Both etymological layers of the vocabulary — the IE and the specifically Germanic layer — are native words. In addition to native words the OG languages share some borrowings made from other lan­guages. Some of the early borrowings are found in all or most languages of the group; probably they were made at the time when the Germanic tribes lived close together as a single speech community, that is in Late PG. It is known that the name of the metal iron was borrowed from the Celtic languages in Late PG; cf. Celt isarno, Gt eisarn, O Icel isarn, OE isen, iren. (The Teutons may have learnt the processing of iron from the Celts.) A large number of words must have been borrowed from Latin prior to the migration of West Germanic tribes to Britain. These words reflect the contacts' of the Germanic tribes with Rome and the influence of the Roman civilisation on their life; they mostly refer to trade and warfare; e.g.:

L pondō, Gt pund, O Icel pund, OE pund, NE pound L prunus, O Icel plóma, OE plume, NE plum L strata via, OHG strâza, OS strâta, OE stræt, NE street (For a more detailed discussion of early Latin borrowings see Ch. X.)

QUESTIONS AND ASSIGNMENTS

1. Name the closest linguistic relations of English.

2.Account for the following place-names: Germany, Saxony, Bavaria, Anglia, Thuringia, Swabia, Gothenburg, Gothland, Burgundy, Allemagne (Frfor Germany), Gotha, Jutland, France, Frankfurt, Normandy, Angtesea, England.

3. Analyse the shifting of word stress in word-building and form-building and point out the words which can illustrate the original Ger­manic way of word accentuation: read, reading, re-read, readable; hear v., bearer, unbearable; satisfy, satisfaction, unsatisfactorily; circumstance, circumstantial, circumstantiality.

4. Explain the sound correspondence in the following parallels from Germanic and non-Germanic languages (the sounds are italicized).

R á î ëü OE b a lu ‘mischief’;
R ñ î ëü G S a lz ‘salt’
L g ena OE c in [kin] (NE chin)
L p ecus Gt faih u, OE fe oh(NE fee)
R íà ã îé NE na k en, G na ck t
R ï ðèÿòåëü NE f riend
R ä åðåâî Gt t riu, NE t ree
L d omare NE t ame

5. Analyse the consonant correspondences in the following groups of words and classify the words into Germanic and non-Germanic: foot, pedal, pedestrian; twofold, double, doublet, twin, brotherly, fra­ternal; tooth, dental, dentist; canine, hound; hearty, cordial; three, trinity; decade, decimals, ten; agriculture, acre; agnostic, know; tame, domestic.

6. Why can examples from the Gothic language often be used to illustrate the PG state while OE and OHG examples are less suitable for the purpose?

7. Classify the following Mod E verbs into descendants of the strong verbs and the weak verbs (Note that the PG -ð- became d or t in English): sing, live, rise, look, answer, speak, run, shake, warn.

8. Prove that suppletion is an ancient way of form-building which goes back to the epoch of the PIE parent-language.

9.We can infer a good deal about the culture of the people, their social structure and geographical conditions from the words of their language. What can be reconstructed of the life of the Teutons from the following list of English words, whose cognates are found in other Ger­manic languages: borough, brew, broth, cliff, earl, east, lore, king, knead, north, sea, seal, ship, south, steer, strand, tin, were, west, whale, wheat?


Chapter IV
CHRONOLOGICAL DIVISIONS IN THE HISTORY OF ENGLISH. SHORT SURVEY OF PERIODS (§ 73-78)

§ 73. The historical development of a language is a continuous uninterrupted process without sudden breaks or rapid transformations. Therefore any periodisation imposed on language history by linguists, with precise dates, might appear artificial, if not arbitrary. Yet in all language histories divisions into periods and cross-sections of a certain length, are used for teaching and research purposes. The commonly accepted, traditional periodisation divides English history into three periods: Old English (OE), Middle English (ME) and New English (NE), with boundaries attached to definite dates and historical events affecting the language. OE begins with the Germanic settlement of Britain (5th c.) or with the beginning of writing (7th c.) and ends with the Norman Conquest (1066); ME begins with the Norman Conquest and ends on the introduction of printing (1475), which is the start of the Modern or New English period (Mod E or NE); the New period lasts to the pres­ent day.

The amendments proposed to the traditional periodisation shift the boundary lines or envisage other subdivisions within the main periods: it has been suggested that ME really began at a later date, c. 1150 (A. Baugh), for the effect of the Norman Conquest on the language could not have been immediate; another suggestion was that we should single out periods of transition and subdivide the three main periods into early, classical, and late (H. Sweet). Some authors prefer a division of history by centuries (M. Schlauch) or a division into periods of two hundred years (B. Strang).

It has been noticed that although language history is a slow uninter­rupted chain of events, the changes are not evenly distributed in time: periods of intensive and vast changes at one or many levels may be followed by periods of relative stability. It seems quite probable that the differences in the rate of changes are largely conditioned by the linguistic situation, which also accounts for many other features of language evo­lution. Therefore division into chronological periods should take into account both aspects: external and internal (extra- and intralinguistic). The following periodisation of English history is partly based on the conventional three periods; it subdivides the history of the English language into seven periods differing in linguistic situation and the na­ture of linguistic changes.

§ 74. The first — pre-written or pre-historical — period, which may be termed Early Old English, lasts from the West Germanic invasion of Britain till the beginning of writing, that is from the 5th to the close of the 7th c. It is the stage of tribal dialects of the West Germanic invaders (Angles, Saxons, Jutes and Frisians), which were gradually losing contacts with the related continental tongues. The tribal dialects were used for oral communication, there being no written form of English.

§ 75. The evolution of the language in this period is hypothetical. It has been reconstructed from the written evidence of other Old Ger­manic languages, especially Gothic, and from later OE written records. It was the period of transition from PG to Written OE. Early OE lin­guistic changes, particularly numerous sound changes, marked OE off from PG and from other OG languages.

§ 76. The second historical period extends from the 8th c. till the end of the 11th. The English language of that time is referred to as Old English or Anglo-Saxon; it can also be called Written OE as compared with the pre-written Early OE period. The tribal dialects gradually changed into local or regional dialects. Towards the end of the period the differences between the dialects grew and their relative position altered. They were probably equal as a medium of oral communication, while in the sphere of writing one of the dialects, West Saxon, had gained supremacy over the other dialects (Kentish, Mercian and Northum­brian). The prevalence of West Saxon in writing is tied up with the rise of the kingdom of Wessex to political and cultural prominence.

The language of this historical period is usually described synchronically and is treated as a more or less stable system, though this assumption may be due to scarcity of evidence, Careful examination of OE texts has revealed increasing variation in the 10th and 11th c, which testifies to growing dialectal divergence and the historical instability of the language.

§ 77. OE was a typical OG language, with a purely Germanic vocabulary, and few foreign borrowings; it displayed specific phonetic pe­culiarities, owing to intensive changes which took place in Early OE. As far as grammar is concerned, OE was an inflected or "synthetic" language with a well-developed system of morphological categories, es­pecially in the noun and adjective, and with an elaborate grouping of a|] inflected parts of speech into morphological classes. H. Sweet, a prominent English scholar of the late 19th c, called OE the "period of full endings" in comparison with later periods. The decline of the mor­phological system began in the Northern dialects in the 10th and 11th c.

§ 78. The third period, known as Early Middle English, starts after 1066, the year of the Norman Conquest, and covers the 12th, 13th and half of the 14th c. It was the stage of the greatest dialectal divergence caused by the feudal system and by foreign influences — Scandinavian and French. The dialectal division of present-day English owes its origin to this period of history.

Under Norman rule the official language in England was French, or rather its variety called Anglo-French or Anglo-Norman; it was also the dominant language of literature. There is an obvious gap in the English literary tradition in the 12th c. The local dialects were mainly used for oral communication and were but little employed in writing. Towards the end of the period their literary prestige grew, as English began to displace French in the sphere of writing, as well as in many other spheres. Dialectal divergence and lack of official English made a favourable environment for intensive linguistic change.

§ 79. Early ME was a time of great changes at all the levels of the language, especially in lexis and grammar. English absorbed two layers of lexical borrowings: the Scandinavian element in the North-Eastern area (due to the Scandinavian invasions since the 8th c.) and the French element in the speech of townspeople in the South-East, especially in the higher social strata (due to the Norman Conquest). Phonetic and gram­matical changes proceeded at a high rate, unrestricted by written tradi­tion. Grammatical alterations were so drastic that by the end of the period they had transformed English from a highly inflected language into a mainly analytical one; for the most part, they affected the nom­inal system. Accordingly, the role of syntactical means of word con­nection grew.

§ 80. The fourth period — from the later 14th c. till the end of the 15th — embraces the age of Chaucer, the greatest English medieval writer and forerunner of the English Renaissance. We may call it Late or Classical Middle English. It was the time of the restoration of English to the position of the state and literary language and the time of literary flourishing. The main dialect used in writing and literature was the mixed dialect of London. (The London dialect was originally derived from the Southern dialectal group, but during the 14th c. the southern traits were largely replaced by East Midland traits.) The literary authority of other dialects was gradually overshadowed by the prestige of the Lon­don written language.

In periods of literary efflorescence, like the age of Chaucer, the pat­tern set by great authors becomes a more or less fixed form of language.

Chaucer's language was a recognised literary form, imitated throughout the 15th c. Literary flourishing had a stabilising effect on language, so that the rate of linguistic changes was slowed down. At the same time the written forms of the language developed and improved.

§ 81. The written records of the late 14th and 15th c. testify to the growth of the English vocabulary and to the increasing proportion of French loan-words in English. The phonetic and grammatical structure had incorporated and perpetuated the fundamental changes of the pre­ceding period. Most of the inflections in the nominal system — in nouns, adjectives, pronouns — had fallen together. H. Sweet called Middle English the period of "levelled endings". The verb system was expand­ing, as numerous new analytical forms and verbal phrases on the way to becoming analytical forms were used alongside old simple forms.

§ 82. The fifth period — Early New English — lasted from the in­troduction of printing to the age of Shakespeare, that is from 1475 to c. 1660. The first printed book in English was published by William Caxton in 1475. This period is a sort of transition between two outstanding epochs of literary efflorescence: the age of Chaucer and the age of Shake­speare (also known as the Literary Renaissance).

It was a time of great historical consequence: under the growing capitalist system the country became economically and politically uni­fied; the changes in the political and social structure, the progress of culture, education, and literature favoured linguistic unity. The growth of the English nation was accompanied by the formation of the national English language.

Caxton's English of the printed books was a sort of bridge between the London literary English of the ME period and the language of the Literary Renaissance. The London dialect had risen to prominence as a compromise between the various types of speech prevailing in the coun­try and formed the basis of the growing national literary language.

§ 83. The Early NE period was a time of sweeping changes at all levels, in the first place lexical and phonetic. The growth of the vocab­ulary was a natural reflection of the progress of culture in the new, bourgeois society, and of the wider horizons of man's activity. New words from internal and external sources enriched the vocabulary. Ex­tensive phonetic changes were transforming the vowel system, which resulted, among other things, in the growing gap between the written and the spoken forms of the word (that is, between pronunciation and spelling). The loss of most inflectional endings in the 15th c. justifies the definition "period of lost endings" given by H. Sweet to the NE period. The inventory of grammatical forms and syntactical construc­tions was almost the same as in Mod E, but their use was different. The abundance of grammatical units occurring without any apparent restrictions, or regularities produces an impression of great "freedom of grammatical construction". Perhaps the choice of forms was moti­vated in a given situation, but its reasons are hard to discern today, and they appear to be used in free variation.

§ 84. The sixth period extends from the mid-17th c. to the close of the 18th c. In the history of the language it is often called "the age of normalisation and correctness", in the history of literature — the "neoclassical" age. This age witnessed the establishment of "norms", which can be defined as received standards recognised as correct at the given period. The norms were fixed as rules and prescriptions of correct usage in the numerous dictionaries and grammar-books published at the time and were spread through education and writing.

It is essential that during the 18th c. literary English differentiated into distinct styles, which is a property of a mature literary language. It is also important to note that during this period the English language extended its area far beyond the borders of the British Isles, first of all to North America.

§ 85. Unlike the age of Shakespeare, the neo-classical period dis­couraged variety and free choice in pronunciation, vocabulary and gram­mar. The 18th c. has been called the period of "fixing the pronunciation". The great sound shifts were over and pronunciation was being stabilised. Word usage and grammatical construction were subjected to restriction and normalisation. The morphological system, particularly the verb system, acquired a more strict symmetrical pattern. The formation of new verbal grammatical categories was completed. Syntactical struc­tures were perfected and standardised.

§ 86. The English language of the 19th and 20th c. represents the seventh period in the history of English — Late New English or Modern English. By the 19th c. English had achieved the relative stability typi­cal of an age of literary florescence and had acquired all the properties of a national language, with its functional stratification and recognised standards (though, like any living language, English continued to grow and change). The classical language of literature was strictly distinguished from the local dialects and the dialects of lower social ranks. The dialects were used in oral communication and, as a rule, had no literary tradition; dialect writing was limited to conversations interpolated in books composed in Standard English or to recording folklore.

The 20th c. witnessed considerable intermixture of dialects. The local dialects are now retreating, being displaced by Standard English. The "best" form of English, the Received Standard, and also the regional modified standards are being spread through new channels: the press, radio, cinema and television.

Nevertheless the two dimensions of synchronic functional stratifi­cation of English are as important as before; the horizontal stratifica­tion in Britain applies to modified regional standards and local dialects, while the vertical dimension reflects the social structure of the English-speaking communities.

The expansion of English overseas proceeded together with the growth of the British Empire in the 19th c. and with the increased weight of the United States (after the War of Independence and the Civil War). English has spread to

Date: 2016-11-17; view: 1434; Íàðóøåíèå àâòîðñêèõ ïðàâ; Ïîìîùü â íàïèñàíèè ðàáîòû --> ÑÞÄÀ...



mydocx.ru - 2015-2024 year. (0.007 sec.) Âñå ìàòåðèàëû ïðåäñòàâëåííûå íà ñàéòå èñêëþ÷èòåëüíî ñ öåëüþ îçíàêîìëåíèÿ ÷èòàòåëÿìè è íå ïðåñëåäóþò êîììåð÷åñêèõ öåëåé èëè íàðóøåíèå àâòîðñêèõ ïðàâ - Ïîæàëîâàòüñÿ íà ïóáëèêàöèþ